The Science of Choosing Between Cooperation and Competition

6 min read

Education & Career Trends: February 22
Curated by the Knowledge Team of ICS Career GPS


People continuously adjust how cooperative they are, as the brain constantly monitors and reacts to others’ social behaviour.

  • Excerpts are taken from an article published on psychologytoday.com.

We live in the most peaceful time in our species’ existence and global cooperation has expanded to unprecedented levels. Yet, sometimes cooperation breaks down and we witness unexpected conflicts. Psychologists, economists, and political scientists have long sought to understand when and why people compete rather than cooperate.

A new study that looked at what happens in the brain when people make these decisions revealed fascinating new insights.

Economic Games

Interacting with others is complicated. We have to think about what we want, what other people want, and what setting we are in, and then combine that information to decide how to behave. A key aspect is whether to be friendly or hostile. The complexity of social decisions makes it hard for scientists to understand how people choose between cooperating or competing.

To solve this, scientists created games which simplify the interactions people have. In these games, players can get money, but the amount depends on both what they and other players choose.

A classic of this type of “economic game” that probes people’s decisions about whether to cooperate is the Prisoner’s Dilemma. It goes like this: Two prisoners are interrogated by the police in separate rooms about a crime they have committed together, but for which the investigators have no evidence. They might get away with it if they both cooperate and refuse to confess. However, if one of the prisoners betrays their accomplice, the one who chooses to cooperate will be heavily punished, while the “defector” will walk away freely. Defecting can therefore seem appealing. Yet, there is a risk: If both accuse each other, investigators will send both to prison. This story became a game for two players. Both win some money if they each cooperate but are tempted to betray their partner to get more money (at the expense of the partner). However, if both players betray each other, neither receives any money.

Researchers have shown that people usually choose to cooperate in the Prisoner’s Dilemma, even when they know they’ll never meet the other player again. When playing multiple times with the same person, players tend to retaliate after occasional betrayals, which in the long term also reduces the chances of further betrayals. 

The prisoner’s dilemma has been really powerful to help researchers understand many different aspects of what makes people choose to cooperate, but it has limits. People always have to choose between only two options: cooperate or not. But in everyday life, things are not that simple. If a colleague is being difficult and then asks for help, we might not want to “fully” cooperate, but we also might not be too competitive. We might be a little bit cooperative to maintain harmony at work, but not expend too much energy being really helpful. How do people decide how cooperative to be? And how do they adjust their cooperation over time?

Measuring continuous changes in cooperation: The Space Dilemma

In a recent study published in Nature Communications, 54 volunteers played a new game in which they could be fully cooperative, fully competitive, or somewhere in between. We did this by getting two players to make decisions about where to position themselves in a virtual two-dimensional space where a reward would suddenly land. The person who was closest to the reward location won that round and the closer that winner was, the more money they received. Where the reward landed in space was completely random. That meant players who put themselves in the middle were being competitive as they increased their chances of winning. However, the actual amount they won on average would usually be low, as the reward would often land far from the middle.

On the other hand, the two players could cooperate by putting themselves at opposite ends of the space.

In this case, each person’s chances of winning an individual round go down but when they do win, it will be a larger amount so they will earn more money across many rounds of the game. As in the Prisoner’s Dilemma, this only works if both cooperate. If one goes to one end while the other tries to win in the middle, the latter will win much more often at the expense of the former. In each round, participants put themselves somewhere in the space, but they did this before seeing where the other player went. As participants’ levels of cooperation or competitiveness were measured by their location in space, we called our new game the “Space Dilemma.” We created different versions of the game, discouraging cooperation by gradually increasing the amount of money given to the winner and, conversely, the punishment for the loser.

People constantly adjust how cooperative they are

People were generally cooperative, but they became increasingly competitive when we boosted the amount they would lose if they got betrayed. They also constantly adjusted their response to the other players’ level of cooperation. We used a mathematical model to calculate how surprised a player would be at the other person’s position. We found that even when the position of the other player was just a little more competitive than expected, it led people to adjust their own position in the next round, making themselves more competitive.

One of the two participants played the game while we scanned their brains. We found that an area called the right temporal-parietal junction (TPJ) signalled how surprised players were at the other person’s level of cooperation. Parts of the medial prefrontal and cingulate cortex – brain areas linked to both making decisions and understanding others — put the surprise in the context of how much money could be lost if they got betrayed, and guided decisions about how cooperative to be next time.

This study shows how people continuously adjust how cooperative they are, as the brain constantly monitors and reacts to others’ social behaviour. More generally, understanding what facilitates cooperative behaviours is essential for increasing people’s well-being and ultimately reducing international conflicts and increasing peace.


Have you checked out yesterday’s blog yet

Top 10 Traits of Creative Entrepreneurs


(Disclaimer: The opinions expressed in the article mentioned above are those of the author(s). They do not purport to reflect the opinions or views of ICS Career GPS or its staff.)

Like this post? For more such helpful articles, click on the button below and subscribe FREE to our blog.


Download our mobile app, ICS Career GPS, a one-stop career guidance platform.

One Reply to “The Science of Choosing Between Cooperation and Competition”

Leave a Reply